Last week I was in Oslo, invited by the organising committee of Eurodoc2017, to give an introductory talk on Open Science [1]. One thing that became apparent during this two-day event was that, although irresistibly trendy, Open Science remains an elusive concept. Many continue to confuse Open Science with Open Access, not to mention that almost everyone still thinks Open Access is equivalent to publishing in open access journals.
In this series of posts, I will discuss a few issues that will hopefully help clarify the meaning of Open Science, why is it important, and how individual scientists can make a difference. I will start by offering my definition of Science, its purpose, and the correct approach to maximise its benefits.
An open-minded scientist does not take any predictive model (i.e., an hypothesis about how nature works) for granted but rather regards theories and models merely as sets of metaphors used to understand and communicate observable effects. He or she is always prepared to modify or completely abandon any given theory if it contradicts the available data. In other words, an open-minded scientist, aiming to reduce uncertainty at all costs, has no investment in the outcome of an experiment. A theory is valid only as long as it can account for all the observed data regardless of individual preferences. Scepticism, on the other, means that in order to be taken seriously, data should be validated through numerous and careful replications. Only data that survive the scrutiny of repeated experimentation can be used to strengthen, modify or abandon existing assumptions about the nature of reality.
The appreciation of science as an uncertainty reduction mechanism driven by open-minded scepticism can help us define Open Science as any practice that facilitates and encourages open-mindedness and scepticism. It also allows us to better comprehend the true problem with the existing academic publishing model. The issue is not that publishers make ridiculous amounts of money for little or zero added value. It is the fact that the current journal-based system does not reward open-minded scepticism. First, to survive in academia, scientists need to publish as many articles as possible, preferably in high-impact prestigious journals. This requires, first and foremost, to keep journal editors and reviewers happy and content. Therefore, individual preferences for theories and models that helped establish successful academic careers become a key factor in deciding what to study and under which perspective. Second, since scientists desperately need to publish, they have a clear investment in the outcome of their experiments, which frequently leads to the deliberate manipulation of study designs and analysis methods to achieve the desired p<.05 [4]. Third, scepticism is also discouraged as journals are usually looking for success stories with significant and groundbreaking findings instead of replication studies and null results. And fourth, you cannot obviously be a productive open-minded scepticist without open access to all data, including articles, raw data, and software code.
Most researchers today, especially those at the beginning of their careers, want to be scientists but are forced to be academics. It is more crucial than ever to change the environment to reward open-minded scepticism and thus bridge the enormous gap between science and academia. Evolution teaches us that profound environmental changes can come from subtle but constant pressure. Grassroots movements of independent scientists like Euroscience, Eurodoc and Open Scholar, will continue to exert constant pressure towards the correct direction, keeping at the same time alive the hope that top-down processes, instigated by governments and institutions, will help establish the appropriate incentives to promote honest scientific enquiry, even against the interests of the academic publishing industry.
Notes
[1] You can download the slides of my presentation here.
[2] This, in brief, is the postulation of the free-energy principle, also known as the theory of the Bayesian brain or predictive coding. For more information see: Friston, Karl. “The Free-Energy Principle: A Unified Brain Theory?” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11, no. 2 (2010): 127–38.
[3] A term borrowed from physicist and consciousness researcher Tom Campbell, author of “My Big Theory of Everything”.
[4] To become an expert in “p-hacking” I suggest you start with these two helpful resources: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Data_dredging and http://shinyapps.org/apps/p-hacker/